Apperly,, I. A., & Butterfill,, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief‐like states? Psychological Review, 116(4), 953–970. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016923
Baillargeon,, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3½‐and 4½‐month‐old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 655–664. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.655
Bard,, K. A., & Leavens,, D. A. (2009). Socio‐emotional factors in the development of joint attention in human and ape infants. In L. Röska‐Hardy, & E. M. Neumann‐Held, (Eds.), Learning from animals? Examining the nature of human uniqueness (pp. 89–104). London, England: Psychology Press.
Barone,, P., Bedia,, M. G., & Gomila,, A. (2020). A minimal turing test: Reciprocal sensorimotor contingencies for interaction detection. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14(March), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00102
Barone,, P., Corradi,, G., & Gomila,, A. (2019). Infants` performance in spontaneous‐response false belief tasks: A review and meta‐analysis. Infant Behavior %26 Development, 57, 101350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101350
Bennett,, J. (1978). Some remarks about concepts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 557–560. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076573
Burge,, T. (2018). Do infants and nonhuman animals attribute mental states? Psychological Review, 125(3), 409–434. http://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000091
Burnside,, K., Ruel,, A., Azar,, N., & Poulin‐Dubois,, D. (2018). Implicit false belief across the lifespan: Non‐replication of an anticipatory looking task. Cognitive Development, 46(February 2017), 4–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.006
Burnside,, K., Severdija,, V., & Poulin‐Dubois,, D. (2020). Infants attribute false beliefs to a toy crane. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12887. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12887
Buttelmann,, D., Buttelmann,, F., Carpenter,, M., Call,, J., & Tomasello,, M. (2017). Great apes distinguish true from false beliefs in an interactive helping task. PLoS One, 12(4), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173793
Buttelmann,, D., Carpenter,, M., Call,, J., & Tomasello,, M. (2007). Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science, 10(4), 31–38. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00630.x
Buttelmann,, D., Carpenter,, M., & Tomasello,, M. (2009). Eighteen‐month‐old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112(2), 337–342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006
Buttelmann,, D., Over,, H., Carpenter,, M., & Tomasello,, M. (2014). Eighteen‐month‐olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected‐contents task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 120–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.002
Buttelmann,, D., Schütte,, S., Carpenter,, M., Call,, J., & Tomasello,, M. (2012). Great apes infer others` goals based on context. Animal Cognition, 15(6), 1037–1053. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0528-4
Buttelmann,, F., Suhrke,, J., & Buttelmann,, D. (2015). What you get is what you believe: Eighteen‐month‐olds demonstrate belief understanding in an unexpected‐identity task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 131, 94–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.009
Butterfill,, S. A., & Apperly,, I. A. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind %26 Language, 28(5), 606–637. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12036
Call,, J., & Tomasello,, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192.
Carpendale,, J. I., & Lewis,, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children`s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(1), 79–96. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x04000032
Carpenter,, M., Nagell,, K., & Tomasello,, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1–143.
Carruthers,, P. (2013). Mindreading in infancy. Mind %26 Language, 28(2), 141–172. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12014
Clements,, W. A., & Perner,, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9(4), 377–395. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4
Colombo,, J., & Mitchell,, D. W. (2009). Infant visual habituation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(2), 225–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2008.06.002
Crivello,, C., & Poulin‐Dubois,, D. (2018). Infants` false belief understanding: A non‐replication of the helping task. Cognitive Development, 46, 51–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.10.003
De Jaegher,, H., Di Paolo,, E., & Gallagher,, S. (2010). Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 441–447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009
de Villiers,, J. G. (2007). The interface of language and theory of mind. Lingua, 117(11), 1858–1878. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2006.11.006
de Villiers,, J. G., & de Villiers,, P. A. (2003). Language for thought: Coming to understand false beliefs. In D. Gentner, & S. Goldin‐Meadow, (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 335–384). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
de Villiers,, P. A. (2005). The role of language in theory‐of‐mind development: What deaf children tell us. In J. W. Astington, & J. Baird, (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp. 266–296). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Dennett,, D. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 568–570. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076664
Dörrenberg,, S., Rakoczy,, H., & Liszkowski,, U. (2018). How (not) to measure infant theory of mind: Testing the replicability and validity of four non‐verbal measures. Cognitive Development, 46, 12–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.01.001
Eilan,, N., Hoerl,, C., McCormack,, T., & Roessler,, J. (Eds.). (2005). Joint attention: Communication and other minds: Issues in philosophy and psychology. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.
Fizke,, E., Butterfill,, S., van de Loo,, L., Reindl,, E., & Rakoczy,, H. (2017). Are there signature limits in early theory of mind? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 209–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.05.005
Gallagher,, S. (2001). The practice of mind. Theory, simulation or primary interaction? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5–7), 83–108.
Gliga,, T., Senju,, A., Pettinato,, M., Charman,, T., & Johnson,, M. H. (2014). Spontaneous belief attribution in younger siblings of children on the autism spectrum. Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 903–913. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034146
Gómez,, J. C. (1996). Second person intentional relations and the evolution of social understanding. Behavioural and Brian Science, 19(1), 129–130. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00041881
Gómez,, J. C. (1998). Are apes persons? The case for primate intersubjectivity. Etica %26 Animali, 9, 51–63.
Gómez,, J. C. (2005). Joint attention and the sensorimotor notion of subject: Insights from apes, normal children, and children with autism. In N. Eilan,, C. Hoerl,, & T. McCormack, (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds (pp. 65–84). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Gómez,, J. C. (2010). The emergence of eye contact as an intersubjective signal in an infant gorilla: Implications for models of early social cognition. Acción Psicológica, 7(2), 35–43. https://doi.org/10.5944/ap.7.2.213
Gomila,, A. (2001). La perspectiva de segunda persona: mecanismos mentales de la intersubjetividad. Contrastes, 6, 65–86. https://doi.org/10.24310/Contrastescontrastes.v0i0.1448
Gomila,, A. (2002). La perspectiva de segunda persona de la atribución mental. Azafea: Revista de Filosofía, 4, 123–138 Retrieved from http://revistas.usal.es/index.php/0213-3563/article/view/3719
Gomila,, A. (2008). La dimensión moral de la perspectiva de segunda persona. In D. Pérez, & L. Fernandez, (Eds.), Cuestiones Filosóficas: ensayos en honor de Eduardo Rabossi (pp. 155–173). Buenos Aires, Argentina: Ed. Catálogos.
Gomila,, A. (2012). Verbal minds. Language and the architecture of cognition. London: Elsevier.
Grosse Wiesmann,, C., Friederici,, A. D., Disla,, D., Steinbeis,, N., & Singer,, T. (2018). Longitudinal evidence for 4‐year‐olds` but not 2‐ and 3‐year‐olds` false belief‐related action anticipation. Cognitive Development, 46, 58–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007
Grosse Wiesmann,, C., Friederici,, A. D., Singer,, T., & Steinbeis,, N. (2017). Implicit and explicit false belief development in preschool children. Developmental Science, 20(5), e12445. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12445
Haman,, M. (2019). Developing theory of mind twenty‐five years after the publication of “Z Badań Nad Kompetencją Komunikacyjną Dziecka”(Edited by B. Bokus and M. Haman). Psychology of Language and Communication, 23(1), 105–136. https://doi.org/10.2478/plc-2019-0006
Harman,, G. (1978). Studying the chimpanzee`s theory of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 576–577. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076743
He,, Z., Bolz,, M., & Baillargeon,, R. (2011). False‐belief understanding in 2.5‐year‐olds: Evidence from violation‐of‐expectation change‐of‐location and unexpected‐contents tasks. Developmental Science, 14(2), 292–305. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00980.x
Heider,, F., & Simmel,, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57(2), 243–259. https://doi.org/10.2307/1416950
Heyes,, C. (2014). False belief in infancy: A fresh look. Developmental Science, 17(5), 647–659. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12148
Hobson,, P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove, UK: Lawrance Erlbaum Associates.
Jacob,, P. (2013). A puzzle about belief‐ascription. In B. Kaldis, (Ed.), Mind and society: Cognitive science meets the philosophy of the social sciences. Synthese Philosophy Library. Berlin, Germany: Springers.
Jacob,, P. (2020). What do false‐belief tests show? Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 11, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-019-00442-z
Kano,, F., Krupenye,, C., Hirata,, S., Tomonaga,, M., & Call,, J. (2019). Great apes use self‐experience to anticipate an agent`s action in a false‐belief test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116(42), 20904–20909. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1910095116
Kellman,, P. J., & Spelke,, E. S. (1983). Perception of partly occluded objects in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 15(4), 483–524. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(83)90017-8
Király,, I., Oláh,, K., Csibra,, G., & Kovács,, Á. M. (2018). Retrospective attribution of false beliefs in 3‐year‐old children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 201803505–201811482. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1803505115
Knudsen,, B., & Liszkowski,, U. (2012a). 18‐month‐olds predict specific action mistakes through attribution of false belief, not ignorance, and intervene accordingly. Infancy, 17(6), 672–691. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00105.x
Knudsen,, B., & Liszkowski,, U. (2012b). Eighteen‐ and 24‐month‐old infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes. Developmental Science, 15(1), 113–122. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01098.x
Krupenye,, C., & Call,, J. (2019). Theory of mind in animals: Current and future directions. WIREs Cognitive Science, 10(6), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1503
Krupenye,, C., Kano,, F., Hirata,, S., Call,, J., & Tomasello,, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110–114. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf8110
Krupenye,, C., Kano,, F., Hirata,, S., Call,, J., & Tomasello,, M. (2017). A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes` performance in a false belief task inanimate control. Communicative %26 Integrative Biology, 10(4), e1343771. https://doi.org/10.1080/19420889.2017.1343771
Krupenye,, C., Tan,, J., & Hare,, B. (2018). Bonobos voluntarily hand food to others UT not toys or tools. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1886), 20181536. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.1536
Kulke,, L., Johannsen,, J., & Rakoczy,, H. (2019). Why can some implicit theory of mind tasks be replicated and others cannot? A test of mentalizing versus submentalizing accounts. PLoS One, 14(3), e0213772. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213772
Kulke,, L., Reiß,, M., Krist,, H., & Rakoczy,, H. (2018). How robust are anticipatory looking measures of theory of mind? Replication attempts across the life span. Cognitive Development, 46, 97–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.09.001
Leavens,, D., & Racine,, T. P. (2009). Joint attention in apes and humans: are humans unique? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16(6–7), 240–267.
Leavens,, D. A., & Bard,, K. A. (2011). Environmental influences on joint attention in great apes: Implications for human cognition. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 10(1), 9–31. https://doi.org/10.1891/1945–8959.10.1.9
Leslie,, A. M. (2005). Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 459–462. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.002
Low,, J., Apperly,, I. A., Butterfill,, S. A., & Rakoczy,, H. (2016). Cognitive architecture of belief reasoning in children and adults: A primer on the two‐systems account. Child Development Perspectives, 10(3), 184–189. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12183
Low,, J., Drummond,, W., Walmsley,, A., & Wang,, B. (2014). Representing how rabbits quack and competitors act: Limits on preschoolers` efficient ability to track perspective. Child Development, 85(4), 1519–1534. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12224
Low,, J., & Perner,, J. (2012). Implicit and explicit theory of mind: State of the art. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02074.x
Low,, J., & Watts,, J. (2013). Attributing false beliefs about object identity reveals a signature blind spot in humans` efficient mrind‐reading system. Psychological Science, 24, 305–311. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612451469
Luo,, Y. (2011). Do 10‐month‐old infants understand others` false beliefs? Cognition, 121(3), 289–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.011
Maye,, A., Isern‐Mas,, C., Barone,, P., & Michael,, J. A. (2017). Sensorimotor accounts of joint attention. Scholarpedia, 12(2), 42361. https://doi.org/10.4249/scholarpedia.42361
Meltzoff,, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re‐enactment of intended acts by 18‐month‐old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838–850. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838
Meristo,, M., Morgan,, G., Geraci,, A., Iozzi,, L., Hjelmquist,, E., Surian,, L., & Siegal,, M. (2012). Belief attribution in deaf and hearing infants. Developmental Science, 15(5), 633–640. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01155.x
Moore,, C., & Barresi,, J. (2017). The role of second‐person information in the development of social understanding. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1667. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01667
Moriguchi,, Y., Ban,, M., Osanai,, H., & Uchiyama,, I. (2018). Relationship between implicit false belief understanding and role play: Longitudinal study. The European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(2), 172–183. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2017.1280022
Murray,, L., & Trevarthen,, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between 2‐month‐olds and their mothers. In T. M. Field, & N. A. Fox, (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Oktay‐Gür,, N., Schulz,, A., & Rakoczy,, H. (2018). Children exhibit different performance patterns in explicit and implicit theory of mind tasks. Cognition, 173, 60–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.001
Onishi,, K. H., & Baillargeon,, R. (2005). Do 15‐month‐old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1107621
Pérez,, D., & Gomila,, A. (2021). Social cognition and the second person in human interaction. London: Routledge.
Perner,, J., & Ruffman,, T. (2005). Infants` insight into the mind: How deep? Science, 308(5719), 214–216. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1111656
Poulin‐Dubois,, D., Polonia,, A., & Yott,, J. (2013). Is false belief skin‐deep? The agent`s eye status influences infants` reasoning in belief‐inducing situations. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(1), 87–99. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2011.608198
Poulin‐Dubois,, D., & Yott,, J. (2014). Fonctions exécutives et théorie de l`esprit chez le jeune enfant: une relation réciproque? Psychologie Française, 59(1), 59–69. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2013.11.002
Poulin‐Dubois,, D., & Yott,, J. (2018). Probing the depth of infants` theory of mind: Disunity in performance across paradigms. Developmental Science, 21(4), e12600. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12600
Powell,, L. J., Hobbs,, K., Bardis,, A., Carey,, S., & Saxe,, R. (2018). Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands. Cognitive Development, 46, 40–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.10.004
Premack,, D., & Woodruff,, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512
Priewasser,, B., Rafetseder,, E., Gargitter,, C., & Perner,, J. (2018). Helping as an early indicator of a theory of mind: Mentalism or teleology? Cognitive Development, 46, 69–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002
Quine,, W. V. (1951). Two dogmas of empiricism. The Philosophical Review, 60(1), 20–43. https://doi.org/10.2307/2181906
Rakoczy,, H., Bergfeld,, D., Schwarz,, I., & Fizke,, E. (2015). Explicit theory of mind is even more unified than previously assumed: Belief ascription and understanding aspectuality emerge together in development. Child Development, 86(2), 486–502. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12311
Redcay,, E., & Schilbach,, L. (2019). Using second‐person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(8), 495–505. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0179-4
Reddy,, V. (1996). Omitting the second person in social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(1), 140–141. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00041996
Reddy,, V. (2008). How infants know minds, Cambrige, MA: Harvard University Press.
Reddy,, V., Markova,, G., & Wallot,, S. (2013). Anticipatory adjustments to being picked up in infancy. PLoS One, 8(6), e65289. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065289
Reddy,, V., & Morris,, P. (2004). Participants don`t need theories: Knowing minds in engagement. Theory %26 Psychology, 14(5), 647–665. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354304046177
Sabbagh,, M., & Bowman,, L. C. (2018). Theory of mind. In J. Wixted, (Ed.), Stevens` handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience (pp. 1–39). New York, NY: John Wiley %26 Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119170174.epcn408
Samuelson,, L. K., Smith,, L. B., Perry,, L. K., & Spencer,, J. P. (2011). Grounding word learning in space. PLoS One, 6(12), e28095. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028095
Schilbach,, L., Timmermans,, B., Reddy,, V., Costall,, A., Bente,, G., Schlicht,, T., & Vogeley,, K. (2013). Toward a second‐person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(04), 393–414. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000660
Schöner,, G., & Thelen,, E. (2006). Using dynamic field theory to rethink infant habituation. Psychological Review, 113(2), 273–299. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.273
Schuwerk,, T., Priewasser,, B., Sodian,, B., & Perner,, J. (2018). The robustness and generalizability of finding on spontaneous false belief sensitivity: A replication attempt. Royal Society Open Science, 5, 172273. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.172273
Schwitzgebel,, E. (2019). Belief. In Edward N. Zalta, (ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford: Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. Retrieved from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2015/entries/belief/
Scott,, R. M., & Baillargeon,, R. (2009). Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about object identity at 18 months. Child Development, 80(4), 1172–1196. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01324.x
Scott,, R. M., & Baillargeon,, R. (2017). Early false‐belief understanding. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(4), 237–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TICS.2017.01.012
Scott,, R. M., Baillargeon,, R., Song,, H., & Leslie,, A. M. (2010). Attributing false beliefs about non‐obvious properties at 18 months. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 366–395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.09.001
Scott,, R. M., Richman,, J. C., & Baillargeon,, R. (2015). Infants understand deceptive intentions to implant false beliefs about identity: New evidence for early mentalistic reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 82, 32–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.08.003
Scotto,, C. (2002). Interacción y atribución mental: la perspectiva de la segunda persona. Análisis Filosófico, 2, 135–151.
Senju,, A., Southgate,, V., Snape,, C., Leonard,, M., & Csibra,, G. (2011). Do 18‐month‐olds really attribute mental states to others? A critical test. Psychological Science, 22(7), 878–880. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611411584
Siposova,, B., & Carpenter,, M. (2019). A new look at joint attention and common knowledge. Cognition, 189, 260–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.03.019
Sodian,, B., Licata,, M., Kristen‐Antonow,, S., Paulus,, M., Killen,, M., & Woodward,, A. (2016). Understanding of goals, beliefs, and desires predicts morally relevant theory of mind: A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 87(4), 1221–1232. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12533
Song,, H., & Baillargeon,, R. (2008). Infants` reasoning about others` false perceptions. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1789–1795. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013774
Song,, H., Onishi,, K. H., Baillargeon,, R., & Fisher,, C. (2008). Can an agent`s false belief be corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18‐month‐old infants. Cognition, 109(3), 295–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.008
Southgate,, V., Chevallier,, C., & Csibra,, G. (2010). Seventeen‐month‐olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others? Referential communication. Developmental Science, 13(6), 907–912. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00946.x
Southgate,, V., Senju,, A., & Csibra,, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2‐year‐olds. Psychological Science, 18(7), 587–592. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x
Surian,, L., Caldi,, S., & Sperber,, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13‐month‐old infants. Psychological Science, 18(7), 580–586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x
Surian,, L., & Franchin,, L. (2020). On the domain‐specificity of the mechanisms underpinning spontaneous anticipatory looks false‐belief tasks. Developmental Science, 23, e12955. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12955
Surian,, L., & Geraci,, A. (2012). Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 30–44. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02046.x
Tauzin,, T., & Gergely,, G. (2018). Communicative mind‐reading in preverbal infants. Scientific Reports, 8, 9534. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-27804-4
Tauzin,, T., & Gergely,, G. (2019). Variability of signal sequences in turn‐taking exchanges induces agency attribution in 10.5‐mo‐olds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116, 201816709–201815446. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1816709116
Thelen,, E., Schöner,, G., Scheier,, C., & Smith,, L. B. (2001). The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01003910
Thoermer,, C., Sodian,, B., Vuori,, M., Perst,, H., & Kristen,, S. (2012). Continuity fro m an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 172–187. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x
Tomasello,, M. (2018). How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(34), 8491–8498. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1804761115
Träuble,, B., Marinovi,, V., & Pauen,, S. (2010). Early theory of mind competencies: Do infants understand others` beliefs? Infancy, 15(4), 434–444. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00025.x
Trevarthen,, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa, (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321–347). London, England: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen,, C. (1998). The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In S. Bråten, (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 15–46). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen,, C., & Hubley,, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock, (Ed.), Action gesture and symbol (pp. 183–229). London, England: Academic Press.
Tronick,, E., Als,, H., Adamson,, L., Wise,, S., & Brazelton,, T. B. (1978). The infant`s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face‐to‐face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17(1), 1–13. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138(09)62273-1
Wang,, B., Hadi,, N. S. A., & Low,, J. (2015). Limits on efficient human mindreading: Convergence across Chinese adults and Semai children. British Journal of Psychology, 106(4), 724–740. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12121
Wang,, L. U., & Leslie,, A. M. (2016). Is implicit theory of mind the ‘real deal’? The own‐belief/true‐belief default in adults and young preschoolers. Mind %26 Language, 31(2), 147–176. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12099
Wellman,, H. M., Cross,, D., & Watson,, J. (2001). Meta analysis of theory of mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655–684. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
Wimmer,, H., & Perner,, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children`s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5
Woodward,, A. L. (2009). Infants` grasp of others` intentions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(1), 53–57. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01605.x
Yamamoto,, S., Humle,, T., & Tanaka,, M. (2012). Chimpanzees` flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics` goals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(9), 3588–3592. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1108517109
Yott,, J., & Poulin‐Dubois,, D. (2012). Breaking the rules: Do infants have a true understanding of false belief? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 156–171. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02060.x
Yott,, J., & Poulin‐Dubois,, D. (2016). Are infants` theory of mind abilities well integrated? Implicit understanding of intentions, desires, and beliefs. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17(5), 683–698. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2015.1086771
Zawidzki,, T. W. (2011). How to interpret infant socio‐cognitive competence. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(3), 483–497.
Zmyj,, N., Prinz,, W., & Daum,, M. M. (2015). Eighteen‐month‐olds` memory interference and distraction in a modified A‐not‐B task is not associated with their anticipatory looking in a false‐belief task. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 857. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00857